Narzędzia osobiste
Jesteś w: Start Groups Strefa dla członków PTKr Filozofia przyrody 2005 Nicole Rempel, "Behavioral Modifications in Insects Induced by Parasites and Pathogens"

Nicole Rempel, "Behavioral Modifications in Insects Induced by Parasites and Pathogens"

http://www.colostate.edu/Depts/Entomology/courses/en507/papers_1997/rempel.html

Behavioral Modifications in Insects Induced by Parasites and Pathogens

Nicole Rempel

[email protected]
Abstract
> <br> Behavioral modifications have been observed in many insects as a result of parasitism or
> pathogens.&nbsp; Altered behaviors can include changes in activities which result in the insect being more <br> conspicuous to predation.  The insect may also alter its body temperature, foraging or oviposition time
> and location, or respond to environmental or mate stimuli differently.&nbsp; These alterations in behavior <br> may represent a wide range of underlying mechanisms with many possible outcomes for parasite and
> host (Moore and Gotelli 1990).&nbsp; Induced behavioral alterations could have arisen for the following <br> reasons as given by Horton and Moore (1993):  the modifications could be natural selection benefiting
> the host, natural selection benefiting the parasite or pathogen, or a consequence of pathology benefiting <br> neither the host nor the parasite or pathogen.
> <br> Many hypotheses have been introduced to explain how the modified behaviors evolved.  These
> theories include kin selected-host suicide, enhanced parasite transmission and survival, and host <br> defense.   Kin selected-host suicide, which may be a behavior of bumblebee workers infected with
> parasitoids, results in behavior changes of the host that increase the host's predation risk with the hope <br> of protecting uninfected kin.   The insects rely on inclusive fitness, that is fitness gained through the
> replication of copies of an individual's genes carried in others through the result of their own actions.&nbsp; <br> Enhanced parasite transmission is thought to consist of behaviors induced by the parasite on the host
> which increase the chances of the parasite entering its final host.&nbsp; This type of behavior may be <br> characteristic of several cockroach species whose behavior becomes more conspicuous to predation
> when infected.&nbsp; Parasites may also alter host behavior in order to increase the parasite's chances of <br> survival, as in aposematic caterpillars and aphids infected with their respective parasitoids.   That is,
> the behavioral change may cause the host to be less susceptible to predation, thereby increasing the <br> chances of parasitoid development and survival.   Host defense is best demonstrated by those insects who
> exhibit behavioral fever in response to a pathogen.&nbsp; By choosing microhabitats which are warmer, the <br> host is able to effectively protect itself from the pathogen by inducing physiological fever.  Examples of
> behavioral fever have been documented in the grasshopper, the cricket, the Madagascar cockroach, the <br> tenebrionid beetle, and the house fly.
> <br> Although Poulin (1995) warns against defining these behaviors as adaptations, many of the
> behaviors do appear to have some adaptive value.&nbsp; The following is a review of several articles <br> demonstrating behavioral modifications of insects to parasitic and pathogenic infections.
> <br> Introduction
> <br> Parasites are known to alter the behavior of the animals in which they live.  This behavioral
> alteration can make the host more conspicuous to predators than uninfected individuals are.&nbsp; Altered <br> behaviors can also include choosing different microhabitats, dietary items, and displaying different
> social status, competitive ability, attractiveness to mates, and activity levels from uninfected <br> conspecifics (Moore and Gotelli 1990).  Changes in microhabitat preference in parasitized insects that
> constitute an altered behavior include seeking higher elevations, seeking exposed locations, seeking <br> concealed locations, changes in reaction to light, nocturnal insects displaying diurnal activity, changes
> in temperature regime, or changes in foraging or oviposition sites (Horton and Moore 1993).<br>
> There are many hypotheses as to why parasitized animals would exhibit behavioral changes.&nbsp; The <br> behaviors could be described as (1) beneficial to the parasite,  making the intermediate host more
> susceptible to predation and allowing the parasite to be transmitted to the definitive host.&nbsp; This <br> susceptibility to predation is only advantageous however, if the parasite is in an intermediate host
> which is preyed upon by the final host, and if the parasite has developed to its infective stage.&nbsp; &quot;By far <br> the majority of documented parasite-induced changes in host behavior thought to be parasite
> adaptations are believed to enhance parasite transmission&nbsp; from host to host&quot; (Poulin 1995).&nbsp; The <br> behaviors may also be beneficial to the pathogen.  The changes may cause better dispersal of an air-
> borne pathogen or enhance the pathogens growth rate.&nbsp; (2) The modified behavior may be beneficial to <br> the host.    From the intermediate host's perspective behaviors such as choosing different locations or
> lighting regimes could constitute induced physiological fever (behavioral fever) and exist as an <br> mechanism to fight off the parasite.   Such situations would have arisen by natural selection to benefit
> the host.&nbsp; Behavioral fever has&nbsp; been observed for several insects infected with protozoans, endotoxins, <br> or bacterial pyrogens.  This behavior would be considered host defense (Horton and Moore 1993). 
> Other explanations for altered behavior due to parasitism include kin selected-host suicide.&nbsp; In host <br> suicide the host behaves in such a way as to increase the probability of death by predation in order to
> lower the risk of parasite infection for other members of the host species (Smith Trail 1980).&nbsp; (3) <br> Other modifications of behavior could not be adaptive to the host or the parasite, but rather the
> response to the pathological effects of the parasite (Robb and Reid 1996).<br>
> Changes in host behavior are often reported in scientific literature and are attributed as being <br> adaptive for the parasite or for the host.  Poulin (1995) suggests however that defining an adaptation
> deserves more rigorous criteria than what has been presented in the past.&nbsp; Accordingly, alterations in <br> host behavior following infection can be defined as adaptations only if they meet certain criteria: "(1)
> they must be complex; (2) they must show signs of a purposive design; (3) they are more likely to be <br> adaptations if they have arisen independently in several lineages of hosts or parasites; and (4) they
> must be shown to increase the fitness of either the host or the parasite&quot; (Poulin 1995).&nbsp; Poulin <br> (1995) indicates that few host-parasite relationships display all of these criteria.  Although many
> show a purposive design, few are complex, and the fitness benefits for most are still ambiguous.&nbsp; <br>
> The following is a review of the more recent works in the area of behavioral alterations <br> observed in insects.  This review has been split into two sections: modified behaviors induced by
> parasitism and modified behaviors induced by endotoxins.&nbsp; In each section examples of behavioral <br> modifications are included for a variety of insects.  Also included is a hypothesis on the adaptive value of
> the behavior: beneficial to host (host defense, and kin selected-host suicide),&nbsp; beneficial to parasite, or <br> a result of pathology with no adaptive value to host or parasite.
> <br> Discussion
> <br> Modified behaviors induced by parasitism
> <br> Stamp (1981) observed parasitized and nonparasitized aposematic caterpillars (the Baltimore
> checkerspot Euphydryas phaeton Drury).&nbsp; She examined the level of mortality of the parasitoids <br> (Apanteles euphydryidis Muesebeck) in order to test the hypothesis of host suicide, that parasitized
> caterpillars advertise themselves to their predators which increases the survivorship of their <br> nonparasitized siblings.  For the host suicide hypothesis to hold, certain restrictions would have to be
> met: the parasitized host should be unable to reproduce, kin should be gregarious, possibilities should <br> exist that the developing parasite would eventually parasitize the kin, and the changes in the host
> behavior should decrease the hosts survivorship (Horton and Moore 1993).&nbsp; Her observations did not <br> support a kinship protection-host suicide hypothesis.  Rather, the behavior of the caterpillar seemed to
> be induced by and beneficial to the parasitoid.&nbsp;&nbsp; The caterpillars, by acting differently, were actually <br> increasing their survivorship as well as the survivorship of the parasitoid by allowing the parasitoid
> to escape predation and hyperparasitism.<br>
> The parasitic wasp Aphidius nigripes, an endoparasitoid of the potato aphid Macrosiphum <br> euphorbiae, completes its pupal development within the mummified aphid host (Brodeur and McNeil
> 1989).&nbsp; Brodeur and McNeil (1989) not only showed that parasitized hosts behaved differently than <br> unparasitized hosts did, but that the behavior depended on the developmental stage of the parasitoid. 
> Aphids containing diapausing parasitoids tended to mummify in concealed areas, while aphids containing <br> nondiaposing parasitoids remained on the leaf near food and other possible aphid hosts.  The aphid hosts
> of diaposing parasitoids, which were preparing to overwinter,&nbsp; sought out protected areas, away from <br> possible physiological and mechanical damage.  The diaposing parasitoids were also able to avoid adverse
> climatic conditions and reduce the chances of hyperparasitism by concealing themselves and their host.&nbsp; <br> This shows one of the more clear examples of a parasite altering the host's behavior in order to
> increase its survivorship.<br>
> The altered behaviors observed in several cockroach species also seem to support the parasite <br> survival hypothesis.  Parasitism affected substrate use and activity in the cockroach species studied
> (Moore et al. 1994).&nbsp; Infected male brown cockroaches, Periplaneta brunnea, spent more time on <br> white horizontal surfaces than did uninfected cockroaches (Carmichael and Moore 1991).  Moore and
> Gotelli (1992) observed decreased travel velocity and distance, and increased use of horizontal <br> substrates for two species of cockroach, Periplaneta americana and Blattella germanica, infected with
> the acanthocephalan Moniliformis moniliformis.&nbsp;&nbsp; They attributed three possible factors to the shift in <br> substrate use: cockroaches on horizontal surfaces may be more susceptible to predation (a behavior
> beneficial to the parasite), reduced sexual sensitivity because cockroaches usually stand on vertical <br> surfaces to enhance the ability to contact females (altered sexual selection behavior), and using
> horizontal surfaces may require less energy than clinging on to vertical surfaces (a behavior reflecting <br> the possible pathological constraints on energy level of the host). 
> <br> Moore and Gotelli (1996) also looked at the phylogeny of behavior modifications in several
> cockroach species to further test the possibility that the behavioral changes were adaptive.&nbsp; The fact <br> that phylogenetic analysis of the components of behavioral alterations in many cockroach species to
> infection were not always shared by close relatives was encouraging, according to Moore and Gotelli <br> (1996).  It supports the hypothesis that there exists a possible adaptive value at the species level to
> the altered behavior conferred on either the cockroach or the parasitic acanthocephalan.&nbsp; They maintain <br> that "if behavioral alterations result from physiological disruptions (neurological, hemolymph
> components, and so on) in one cockroach species, we would expect such alterations to be shared more <br> readily by close relatives than distant ones, regardless of the adaptive nature of those alterations in
> every association&quot; (Moore and Gotelli 1996).&nbsp; This corresponds with Poulin's (1995) criteria that <br> behaviors are more likely to be adaptations if they have arisen in several lineages independently.  
> <br> Robb and Reid (1996) were interested in determining whether or not the flour beetle's,
> Tribolium confusum, behavioral modifications were due to pathology caused by the parasite, <br> Hymenolepis diminuta, or if the parasite altered the intermediate host's behavior as an adaptive
> manipulation.&nbsp; Their findings indicated that both mated status and infection affected the survivorship of <br> the host, infected mated females surviving less than both infected virgin beetles and uninfected beetles. 
> Behavior, however, was only altered significantly by infection of the parasite and not by mated status.&nbsp; <br> They concluded that behavior did not appear to be a pathological response to the parasite but rather
> supported the hypothesis of&nbsp; the host's behavior being modified by the parasite.&nbsp; However, in Zuk's <br> (1988) experiment on parasitized crickets, the findings suggest pathological consequences to
> parasitism.&nbsp;&nbsp; Male parasitized crickets produced fewer spermatophores and had lower mating success <br> than uninfected males did.  This is one example of how sexual selection was affected in an insect host due
> to the pathological consequences of the parasite load.<br>
> Bumblebees, Bombus spp., on the other hand appear to have successfully mastered the use of <br> altered behavior for their own advantage.  Mueller and Schmid-Hempel (1993) reported that the
> parasitized worker bumblebees stayed in the field overnight instead of returning to the nest.&nbsp; These <br> workers spent significantly more time in cold areas than did nonparasitized workers.  The cold
> temperatures experienced by the bumblebees retarded parasitoid development and decreased the <br> parasitoid's survival chances.  The parasitized worker's colony benefited from the prolonged foraging in
> the cold night air, and the worker had a prolonged life span as a result of the reduced development rate <br> of the parasitoid.  Poulin (1992) argues that these changes in behaviors of parasitized bumblebee
> workers are likely to be an adaptive response of the host resulting in greater inclusive fitness.&nbsp; He <br> notes that this may be one of the few examples of Smith Trail's (1980) kin selected-host suicide
> hypothesis in practice in nature.<br>
> In certain instances, however, where it has been shown that a parasitized animal could benefit <br> from behavioral modifications, there may be no adaptation.  For example, in nematode-parasitized
> Drosophila high temperatures had deleterious effects on the parasites.&nbsp; But when given a choice of <br> temperature in a thermal gradient, neither species of D. falleni nor D. neotestacea modified their
> behavior in favor of the higher temperature (behavioral fever) (Ballabeni et al. 1995).&nbsp; <br>
> Modified behavior induced by endotoxins<br>
> A host would benefit if, by altering its behavior, it could effectively harm the parasite.&nbsp; Many <br> ectotherms respond to a parasite by changing their microhabitat in such a way as to elevate their own
> body temperature (Horton and Moore 1993).&nbsp;&nbsp; By doing so the animal exhibits behavioral fever.&nbsp; <br> Recent literature on insects infected with endotoxins indicates that the host insect is able to use
> behavioral fever to its advantage against microsporidian protozoan, intracellular prokaryotes, <br> bacterial endotoxins or prostaglandins, and fungus.  The  adaptive value of behavioral fever to pathogens
> can be expressed in three ways:&nbsp; fever may be an adaptation of the host as a defense against the pathogen, <br> a modification of the host by the pathogen to enhance growth, dispersal, and survival of the pathogen, or
> merely a side effect of infection and benefiting neither host not pathogen (Boorstein and Ewald 1987). <br>
> Boorstein and Ewald (1987) inoculated grasshoppers Melanoplus sanguinipes with the <br> microsporidian protozoan Nosema acridophagus and showed that their preferred temperature increased. 
> By maintaining the grasshoppers at both febrile and nonfebrile temperatures they were able to show <br> that the febrile temperatures benefited the infected grasshoppers in survival and growth.  There is a
> cost to the fever, and febrile uninfected animals were negatively affected in growth.&nbsp; However, for an <br> infected animal these costs were outweighed by the benefits.  Infected insects maintained at nonfebrile
> temperatures had significantly lower fecundity, survival, and growth rates than controls.&nbsp; Fever was <br> beneficial to the infected animals in that they lacked significant differences from controls in fecundity,
> survival, and growth.<br>
> Crickets Gryllus bimaculatus exhibit&nbsp; behavioral fever in response to infection with <br> Rickettsiella grylli, a chlamydia-like pathogen (Louis 1986).   When allowed to regulate their own
> temperature in a thermal gradient, infected insects chose higher temperatures than noninfected insects <br> did.  This higher temperature caused the pathogen to degenerate.
> <br> Two different insects display behavioral fever to endotoxins, specifically purified
> lipopolysaccharides isolated from E. Coli.&nbsp; Lipopolysaccharides, components of the cell wall of Gram-<br> negative bacteria (Bronstein and Conner 1984), are potent pyrogens (McClain et al. 1988) that elicit
> a sequence of host-defense responses in animals.&nbsp; Bronstein and Conner (1984) conducted a study of <br> endotoxin-induced behavioral fever in the Madagascar cockroach Gromphadorhina portentosa. 
> Lipopolysaccharide-W was injected into the insect and the mean temperature preference was 3.6 <br> degrees higher than that for control cockroaches.  Also, the tenebrionid beetle Onymacris plana was
> observed by McClain et al. in 1988 to develop behavioral fever in response to lipopolysaccharide <br> injection and sought higher preferred temperatures in a gradient.  To be certain that the body
> temperature was indeed affected by the position of the beetle on the thermal gradient, thermocouples <br> were implanted in the thoracic musculature. 
> <br> A final example of behavioral fever is the modification in temperature preference induced in
> house flies Musca domestica when infected with the fungi Entomophthora muscae (Watson et al. 1992).&nbsp; <br> In experiments where flies were not given a choice of temperature, higher temperatures increased the
> survival period of the infected flies.&nbsp; In thermal gradients the flies preferred the higher temperatures, <br> exhibited behavioral fever, and were able to eliminate the pathogen using heat therapy.
> <br> With few exceptions, ectothermic animals, like endotherms will develop fevers in response to
> injections of endotoxins or other pyrogenic substances as a result of the animal &quot;feeling&quot; cold and <br> selecting a warmer microclimate (Kluger et al. 1996).  Kluger et al. suggests that febrile responses
> are most likely adaptive because if they weren't, than &quot;it would be unlikely that this energetically <br> expensive phenomenon would have persisted for millions of years in so many groups of animals".  With
> the exception of the bumblebee worker, most other insects are harmed by pathogens when their body <br> temperature is lowed and protected when they develop fevers (Kluger et al. 1996).  Kluger et al. also
> indicates that this observation and the fact that fevers are highly regulated, that they follow a <br> relatively common sequence of events within the body, both support the argument that fever has
> evolved as a host defense response.&nbsp; <br>
> Conclusion<br>
> Insects alter their behavior when they are under the stress of infection by parasites or <br> pathogens.  Such behavioral changes include seeking out warmer or cooler temperatures, changing their
> microhabitat, and acting more conspicuously.&nbsp; Many times the behavior will increase the probability <br> that the insect will be preyed upon.  This is often attributed as an adaptation of the parasite to alter its
> host's behavior in order to increase its transmission to a final host.&nbsp; It is also often the case that the <br> host insect will harm the parasite or pathogen by changing its behavior, such as increasing its body
> temperature, as seen in many of the insects infected with endotoxins.&nbsp;&nbsp; Seeking out warmer <br> environments in order to induce a physiological fever constitutes a behavioral fever and has been shown
> to effectively protect the insect from the pathogen.&nbsp; Whether some adaptations favor the parasite's <br> survival or the host's is still unclear in many of the parasite-host relationships.  Further studies are
> necessary, as Poulin (1995) suggests, in order to critically determine whether a behavior is adaptive.&nbsp; <br> It is important to determine the fitness costs of behavioral modification, the phylogeny of the behavior
> among close and distant relatives,&nbsp; and the pathological changes caused by the parasite and the effects <br> that those changes may have on host behavior.
> <br> References
> <br> Ballabeni, P., H. Benway, and J. Jaenike. 1995. Lack of Behavioral Fever in Nematode-Parasitized
> Drosophila. Journal of Parasitology 81(5): 670-674.<br>
> Boorstein, S.M., and P.W. Ewald.&nbsp; 1987. Costs and Benefits of Behavioral Fever Melanoplus sanguinipes <br> Infected By Nosema acridophagus. Physiological Zoology 60(5): 586-595.
> <br> Brodeur J., and J.N. McNeil. 1989. Seasonal Microhabitat Selection by an Endoparasite Through
> Adaptive Modification of Host Behavior. Science 244: 226-228.<br>
> Bronstein, S.M., and W.E. Conner. 1984. Endotoxin-Induced Behavioral Fever in Madagascar <br> Cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa. Journal of Insect Physiology 30(4): 327-330.
> <br> Carmichael, L.M., and J. Moore. 1991. A Comparison of Behavioral Alterations in the Brown Cockroach,
> Periplaneta brunnea, and the American Cockroach, Periplaneta americana, Infected With the <br> Acanthocephalan, Moniliformis moniliformis. Journal of Parasitology 77(6): 931-936.
> <br> Gotelli, N.J., and J. Moore. 1992. Altered Host Behavior in a Cockroach- Acanthocephalan Association. 
> Animal Behavior 43: 949-959.<br>
> Horton, D.R., and J. Moore. 1993. Behavioral Effects of Parasites and Pathogens in Insect Hosts. In: <br> Parasites and Pathogens of Insects (Ed. by N.E. Beckage, S.N. Thompson, and B.A. Federici), pp. 107-
> 124. San Diego: Academic Press.<br>
> Kluger, M.J., W. Kozak, C.A. Conn, L.R. Leon, and D. Soszynski. 1996. The Adaptive Value of Fever. <br> Infectious Disease Clinics of North America 10(1): 1-21.
> <br> Louis, C., M. Jourdan, and M. Cabanac. 1986. Behavioral Fever and Therapy in a Rickettsia-Infected
> Orthoptera. American Journal of Physiology 250: R991-R995. <br>
> McClain, E., P. Magnuson, and S.J. Warner. 1988. Behavioral Fever in a Namib Desert Tenebrionid <br> Beetle, Onymacris plana. Journal of Insect Physiology 34(4): 279-284.
> <br> Moore, J., M. Freehling, and N.J. Gotelli. 1994. Altered Behavior in Two Species of Blattid Cockroaches
> Infected With Moniliformis moniliformis (Acanthocephala). Journal of Parasitology 80(2): 220-223.<br>
> Moore, J. and N.J. Gotelli. 1990. A Phylogenetic Perspective on the Evolution of Altered Behaviors: a <br> Critical Look at the Manipulation Hypothesis. In: Parasitism and Host Behavior (Ed. by C.J. Barnes and
> J.M. Behnke), pp. 193-233. London: Taylor &amp; Francis.<br>
> Moore, J. and N.J. Gotelli. 1996. Evolutionary Patterns of Altered Behavior and Susceptibility in <br> Parasitized Hosts. Evolution 50(2): 807-819.
> <br> Mueller, C.B., and P. Schmid-Hempel. 1993. Exploitation of Cold Temperature as Defense Against
> Parasitoids in Bumblebees. Nature 363: 65-67.<br>
> Poulin, R. 1992. Altered Behavior in Parasitized Bumblebees: Parasite Manipulation or Adaptive <br> Suicide? Animal Behavior 44: 174-176.
> <br> Poulin, R. 1995. "Adaptive" Changes in the Behavior of Parasitized Animals: A Critical Review.
> International Journal of Parasitology 25(12): 1371-1383.<br>
> Robb, T., and M.L. Reid. 1996. Parasite-Induced Changes in the Behavior of Cestode-Infected Beetles: <br> Adaptation or Simple Pathology? Canadian Journal of Zoology 74: 1268-1274.
> <br> Smith Trail, D.R. 1980. Behavioral Interactions Between Parasites and Hosts: Host Suicide and the
> Evolution of Complex Life Cycles. The American Naturalist 116(1): 77-91.<br>
> Stamp, N.E. 1981. Behavior of Parasitized Aposematic Caterpillars: Advantageous to the Parasitoid or <br> the Host? The American Naturalist 118(5): 715-725.
> <br> Watson, D.W., B.A. Mullens, and J.J. Petersen. 1992. Behavioral Fever Response of Musca domestica
> (Diptera: Muscidae) to Infection by Entomophthora muscae (Zygomycetes: Entomophthorales). Journal <br> of Invertebrate Pathology 61: 10-16.
> <br> Zuk, M. 1988. Parasite Load, Body Size, and Age of Wild-Caught Male Field Crickets (Orthoptera:
> Gryllidae): Effects on Sexual Selection.&nbsp; Evolution 42(5): 969-976.<br>
Akcje Dokumentu
« Maj 2024 »
Maj
PnWtŚrCzPtSbNd
12345
6789101112
13141516171819
20212223242526
2728293031